A few months ago, Social Service Providers Aotearoa asked me to review the literature on school breakfast programmes and provide an assessment of whether public funding of school breakfast programmes offered value for money. I spoke on the issue in Wellington and in Christchurch in February. As the government seems to be looking at the Mana Party's proposals around food in schools, it seems worth posting things here as summary.
I was only looking at school breakfast programmes, and so I
can't here comment on school lunch programmes. I'm not sure why we'd expect
results to vary greatly, but it's worth having the caveat.
Anyway, on my best read of the literature, it's hard to make
a case for that we'd get any great benefit from the programmes. Rather, we
often find that they don't even increase the odds that kids eat breakfast at
all. Many shift breakfast from at-home to at-school, but among those who hadn't
bothered with breakfast before the programme, not many wind up starting when
schools provide it. You can then get kids reporting that they're less hungry as
consequence of the programmes, but it's awfully hard to reject that the main
thing going on is that kids are eating at 9 at school instead of at 7 at home
and are consequently less hungry when asked at 11.
You can get some substantial results from school breakfast
programmes in third world countries. But even there we need to watch for
displacement effects: the benefit of the programmes is often the implicit
income subsidy provided. In those cases, we can see evidence
of families cutting back on food expenditures for the kid getting
breakfast at school in favour of spending on the other kids; in the link
provided, there's reasonable crowding out in a UK lunch programme. And if
that's the benefit, cutting a cheque to the families instead just might be
better.
In all the studies, I wish that there were a control group
where the parents were just given cash equivalent to the per-student cost of
putting on the programme. All of these kinds of programmes should be assessed
against that kind of counterfactual to establish whether we're getting benefits
from the programme, or from the implicit income transfer.
Here are a few typical pieces.
Devaney and
Fraker, 1989, found that school breakfast programmes did not increase the
likelihood of kids' eating breakfast at all. It did increase calcium intake and
reduce consumption of cholesterol and iron - breakfasts provided at school
differed from those they'd be getting at home.
Gleason,
1995, similarly found that school breakfast programmes did not influence
the likelihood of students' eating breakfast.
Alderman
and Bundy, 2011, concluded that food in schools isn't a great investment
but could complement other investments - they focused on developing countries.
Bhattacharya,
Currie and Haider, 2006, seems to be the touchstone for those advocating
school breakfast programmes. They found improved nutritional outcomes in blood
serum tests of kids participating in school breakfast programmes compared to
the same kids during school holidays when they weren't getting the school
breakfasts. But they also found no effect on the likelihood of eating
breakfast. And I worry a bit about their identification strategy: because it's
poorer schools who got school breakfast programmes, we might expect that there
could be relevant differences in how parents respond to school holidays that
might affect the difference between school/not school outcomes for reasons
other than the programme.
Waehrer, 2008,
in an unpublished study funded by the USDA's RIDGE programme, found that school
breakfast participation reduced the likelihood of eating
breakfast. We could imagine this happening where the kids don't really want
breakfast anyway, the parents stop making them eat it at home because there's
the programme at school, and then they skip it when they get to school. The
study could have similar identification issues to the Bhattacharya piece noted
above; they identify on weekday-weekend differences, but cohorts might respond
differently to weekends.
Shemilt,
Harvey, Shepstone et al, 2004, found pretty mixed outcomes in a messy
randomised control trial. They wound up abandoning the RCT part of the analysis
and just going for regressions. They found some evidence ofworsened outcomes
of having attended school breakfast programmes on a few behavioural measures,
but I'm again not convinced that they've pinned down causality. What they
seemed most sure of was that school breakfast programmes had kids eating more
fruit, so I guess there's that.
There were a couple of pieces claiming reasonable benefits
from school breakfast programmes too.
Powell,
Walker, et al, 1998, ran a really nice randomised control trial in Jamaica.
Kids in the programme got breakfast, those not in the programme were given a
small piece of orange. So they're able to isolate socialisation effects from
breakfast effects. They found that the treatment group saw small increases in
nutritional status, achievement, and attendance; they suggested that
"greater improvements may occur in more undernourished populations."
I'm not convinced that we're in that category.
Murphy,
Pagano et al (1998) found that moving from selective to universal
school breakfast programmes had some benefits, but also had some odd results.
Before intervention, "hungry and at-risk children were slightly, but not
significantly, more likely to participate in the school breakfast program than
nonhungry children", and that more than half of the hungry and at-risk
kids rarely or never participated in voluntary school breakfast programmes. So
stigma associated with voluntary programmes can substantially affect uptake.
But, when the programmes were made universal, hungry and at-risk kids were only
"somewhat more likely to increase their school breakfast participation
than non-hungry children... although this difference was not statistically
significant." So what do we then make of results showing some improved
average outcomes at school but no particular increase in breakfast-eating among
those who are hungry? I wonder if all the effects here point to that eating
later in the morning rather than earlier is better. I'll talk more about this
below.
Dotter,
2012, finds that universal in-class school breakfasts increase the number
of children eating breakfast at school compared to voluntary programmes that
could have stigma effects, but I couldn't see that the paper measured whether
there was an effect on total breakfast consumption. And while Dotter finds
increased school performance in schools with universal school breakfast
programmes, I can't see how the paper distinguishes between an "eating at
all" and an "eating later" effect. Why does this matter? Imagine
an alternative policy where schools allow a designated morning tea break at
10:30 where kids bring in their own snacks. This would be cheaper than full
school breakfast programmes and just as effective, if the main channel of
effectiveness is having a fuller tummy at the time of instruction because
breakfast was later.
Frisvold,
2012, found that state mandates requiring schools to provide school
breakfast programmes increase availability of those programmes and consequently
increase test scores: the paper reports math score increases of nine percent of
a standard deviation and reading score increases of five percent of a standard
deviation. Again, there is no significant effect on the total days per week
that a student eats breakfast, suggesting substantial displacement of
breakfasts that would otherwise have been eaten at home. The paper claims that
the effect is through a nutrition channel, with kids eating healthier
breakfasts. But I can't see how they're distinguishing the nutrition channel
from my suggested "they're eating later in the morning and so are less
hungry at 11" channel.
So, some bottom lines:
- School breakfast programmes really don't seem to increase the likelihood of that kids eat breakfast at all;
- To the extent that they improve outcomes in some studies, we really can't tell:
- whether the effect is from changing the timing of breakfast, in which case we should instead have a morning tea break;
- whether the effect is any better than just giving those families an equivalent cash transfer.
I spent an hour in Wellington and Christchurch walking
through these findings. I hope we don't throw a pile of money at school
breakfast programmes; the money could well be better spent. That also seemed to
be the conclusion of a New Zealand study: Mhurchu et al, 2012, who
found that the only effect of a randomised control trial of school free
breakfast programmes here was that kids self-reported being less hungry.
Dr Eric Crampton is a senior lecturer in economics at the
University of Canterbury. He blogs at Offsetting Behaviour.
1 comment:
While its known that sex is perhaps the main surviver in holding the human race together, food must come in a very close second. As a general rule a lack of nutrients, minerals and vitamins caused through a faulty diet first manifests in mental disorders. Is it any wonder then that malnourished children may refuse breakfast both at home and at school?
The poverty debate misses a greater problem - the kiwi capitalist confetti shredding machine. When shareholders continually demand more profit then many people get either screwed or very tightly squeezed. The system may deny that the people always deserve the fairest go but the people themselves always tend to think more simply.
About 90 % of us regularly use alcohol yet our system drug deals it to us. A bottle of spirits costs just a few dollars to produce yet is retailed at $40. Tobacco is much worse, its now retailing at, would you believe, $1000000 per tonne (one million dollars/tonne - $1.00/gram). And this when a similar field grown crop like corn sells for about $600/tonne - $0.0006/gram???
Its easy then to see just how the drinkers and smokers and many more unfortunates, are being ripped in all directions and in all too many families this is causing severe hardship. Of course the social purists will continue to cry why don't they give up? yer right.
John P
Post a Comment